How long does it take to become a proficient hunter? Implications for the evolution of extended development and long life span

J Hum Evol. 2006 Nov;51(5):454-70. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2006.05.003. Epub 2006 May 22.

Abstract

Human hunting is arguably one of the most difficult activities common to foraging peoples now and in the past. Children and teenagers have usually been described as incompetent hunters in ethnographies of hunter-gatherers. This paper explores the extent to which adult-level competence is limited more by the constraints of physical capital, or body size, and brain-based capital, or skills and learning. The grandmother hypothesis requires that production is an increasing function of size alone, while the embodied capital model stipulates that production is a function of both size and delayed learning. Tests based on observational, interview, and experimental data collected among Tsimane Amerindians of the Bolivian Amazon suggest that size alone cannot explain the long delay until peak hunting productivity. Indirect encounters (e.g., smells, sounds, tracks, and scat) and shooting of stationary targets are two components of hunting ability limited primarily by physical size alone, but the more difficult components of hunting--direct encounters with important prey items and successful capture--require substantial skill. Those skills can take an additional ten to twenty years to develop after achieving adult body size.

Publication types

  • Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov't
  • Research Support, U.S. Gov't, Non-P.H.S.

MeSH terms

  • Adolescent
  • Adult
  • Age Factors
  • Aged
  • Animals
  • Anthropology, Physical
  • Body Height
  • Body Weight
  • Bolivia
  • Child
  • Humans
  • Indians, South American* / education
  • Indians, South American* / psychology
  • Learning*
  • Longevity / physiology
  • Male
  • Middle Aged
  • Muscle Strength / physiology
  • Predatory Behavior*