Skip to main content
Log in

Cultural Transmission Theory and the Archaeological Record: Providing Context to Understanding Variation and Temporal Changes in Material Culture

  • Published:
Journal of Archaeological Research Aims and scope

Abstract

Cultural transmission (CT) is implicit in many explanations of culture change. Formal CT models were defined by anthropologists 30 years ago and have been a subject of active research in the social sciences in the ensuing years. Although increasing in popularity in recent years, CT has not seen extensive use in archaeological research, despite the quantitative rigor of many CT models and the ability to create testable hypotheses. Part of the reason for the slow adoption, we argue, has been the continuing focus on change in central tendency and mode in archaeology, instead of change in dispersion or variance. Yet archaeological research provides an excellent data source for exploring processes of CT. We review CT research in the anthropological sciences and outline the benefits and drawbacks of this theoretical framework for the study of material culture. We argue that CT can shed much light on our understandings of why material technology changes over time, including explanations of differential rates of change among different technologies. We further argue that transmission processes are greatly affected by the content, context, and mode of transmission and fundamentally structure variation in material culture. Including ideas from CT can provide greater context for explaining and understanding changes in the variation of artifacts over time. Finally, we outline what we feel should be the goals of CT research in archaeology in the coming years.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References cited

  • Aldenderfer, M. A. (1993). Ritual, hierarchy, and change in foraging societies. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 12: 1–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkow, J. H. (1975). Prestige and culture: A biosocial interpretation. Current Anthropology 16: 553–576.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barth, F. (1987). Cosmologies in the Making: A Generative Approach to Cultural Variation in Inner New Guinea, Cambridge University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barth, F. (1990). The guru and the conjurer: Transactions in knowledge and the shaping of culture in Southeast Asia and Melanesia. Man 25: 640–653.

    Google Scholar 

  • Basalla, G. (1988). The Evolution of Technology, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentley, R. A. (2005). Academic copying, archaeology and the English language. Antiquity 80: 196–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentley, R. A., and Maschner, H. D. G. (2000). A growing network of ideas. Fractals 8: 227–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentley, R. A., and Maschner, H. D. G. (2001). Stylistic change as a self-organized critical phenomenon: An archaeological study in complexity. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 8: 35–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentley, R. A., and Shennan, S. J. (2003). Cultural transmission and stochastic network growth. American Antiquity 68: 459–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentley, R. A., Hahn, M. W., and Shennan, S. J. (2004). Random drift and culture change. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 271: 1443–1450.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettinger, R. L., and Eerkens, J. W. (1997). Evolutionary implications of metrical variation in Great Basin projectile points. In Barton, C. M., and Clark, G. A. (eds.), Rediscovering Darwin: Evolutionary Theory and Archaeological Explanation, Archeological Papers No. 7, American Anthropological Association, Arlington, VA, pp. 177–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettinger, R. L., and Eerkens, J. W. (1999). Point typologies, cultural transmission, and the spread of bow-and-arrow technology in the prehistoric Great Basin. American Antiquity 64: 231–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bettinger, R. L., Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1996). Style, function, and cultural evolutionary processes. In Maschner, H. D. G. (ed.), Darwinian Archaeologies, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 133–164.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bikhchandani, S., Hirschleifer, D., and Welch, I. (1998). Learning from the behaviour of others: Conformity, fads and informational cascades. Journal of Economic Perspectives 12: 151–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biro, D., Inoue-Nakamura, N., Tonooka, R., Yamakoshi, G., Sousa, C., and Matsuzawa, T. (2003). Cultural innovation and transmission of tool use in wild chimpanzees: Evidence from field experiments. Animal Cognition 6: 213–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackham, M. (2000). Distinguishing bioturbation and trampling using pottery sherd measures, Tell Fendi, Jordan. Geoarchaeology 15: 469–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boas, F. (1896). The limitations of the comparative method in anthropology. Science 4: 901–907.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bonner, J. T. (1980). The Evolution of Culture in Animals, Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the Evolutionary Process, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1987). The evolution of ethnic markers. Cultural Anthropology 2: 65–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1988). An evolutionary model of social learning: The effects of spatial and temporal variation. In Zentall, T., and Galef, B. G. (eds.), Social Learning: A Psychological and Biological Approach, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 29–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1995). Why does culture increase human adaptability? Ethology and Sociobiology 16: 125–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (2002). Group beneficial norms can spread rapidly in a structured population. Journal of Theoretical Biology 215: 287–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brew, J. O. (1946). Archaeology of Alkali Ridge, Southeastern Utah, Papers of the Peabody Museum of American Archaeology and Ethnology No. 21, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.

  • Brown, C., and Laland, K. N. (2003). Social learning in fishes: A review. Fish and Fisheries 4: 280–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D. T. (1965). Variation and selective retention in socio-cultural evolution. In Barringer, H. R., Blanksten, G. I., and Mack, R. W. (eds.), Social Change in Developing Areas: A Reinterpretation of Evolutionary Theory, Schenkman, Cambridge, MA, pp. 19–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., and Feldman, M. W. (1973). Models for cultural inheritance. I. Group mean and within group variation. Theoretical Population Biology 4: 42–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., and Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A Quantitative Approach, Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., and Feldman, M. W. (1983). Paradox of the evolution of communication and of social interactivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 80: 2017–2021.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cloak, F. T., Jr. (1975). Is a cultural ethology possible? Human Ecology 3: 161–182.

    Google Scholar 

  • Combet, F., Jaussaud, P., and Martin, N. (2005). Estimation of slight speed gaps between signals via the scale transform. Mechanical Systems & Signal Processing 19: 230–257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conkey, M., and Hastorf, C. (eds.) (1990). The Use of Style in Archaeology, University of Cambridge Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coren, S. L., Ward, L. M., and Enns, J. T. (1994). Sensation and Perception, Harcourt Brace, Fort Worth, TX.

  • Cover, T. M., and Thomas, J. A. (1991). Elements of Information Theory, Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cronk, L. (1999). That Complex Whole: Culture and the Evolution of Human Behavior, Westview, Boulder, CO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene, Oxford University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins, R. (1982). The Extended Phenotype, Oxford University Press, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Winter, W. (1997). The beanbag genetics controversy: Towards a synthesis of opposing views of natural selection. Biology and Philosophy 12: 149–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deetz, J. (1965). The Dynamics of Stylistic Change in Arikara Ceramics, Illinois Studies in Anthropology, Urbana.

  • Deetz, J., and Dethlefsen, E. S. (1965). The Doppler-effect and archaeology: A consideration of the spatial aspects of seriation. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology 21: 196–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deetz, J., and Dethlefsen, E. S. (1971). Some social aspects of New England colonial mortuary art. In Brown, J. A. (ed.), Memoirs of the Society for American Archaeology, No. 25, Society for American Archaeology, Washington, DC, pp. 30–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dobres, M. A. (2000). Creativity in human evolution and prehistory. American Antiquity 65: 768–769.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doolittle, W. F., and Sapienza, C. (1980). Selfish genes, the phenotype paradigm and genome evolution. Nature 284: 601–603.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunnell, R. C. (1971). Systematics in Prehistory, Free Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunnell, R. C. (1978). Style and function: A fundamental dichotomy. American Antiquity 43: 192–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunnell, R. C. (1985). Methodological issues in contemporary Americanist archaeology. In Asquith, P. D., and Ketcher, P. (eds.), Proceedings of the 1984 Biennial Meetings of the Philosophy of Science Association, Volume 2, Philosophy of Science Association, East Lansing, MI, pp. 717–744.

  • Dunnell, R. C. (1992). Archaeology and evolutionary science. In Wandsnider, L. (ed.), Quandaries and Quests: Visions of Archaeology’s Future, Occasional Paper No. 20, Center for Archaeological Investigations, Southern Illinois University Press, Carbondale, pp. 209–221.

  • Durham, W. H. (1976). The adaptive significance of cultural behavior. Human Ecology 4: 89–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eerkens, J. W. (2000). Practice makes within 5% of perfect: The role of visual perception, motor skills, and human memory in artifact variation and standardization. Current Anthropology 41: 663–668.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eerkens, J. W., and Bettinger, R. L. (2001). Techniques for assessing standardization in artifact assemblages: Can we scale material variability? American Antiquity 66: 493–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eerkens, J. W., and Lipo, C. P. (2005). Cultural transmission, copying errors, and the generation of variation in material culture in the archaeological record. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24: 316–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feathers, J. K. (1989). Ceramic analysis, variation, and database construction: A selectionist perspective. In Blakely, J. A., and Bennett, W. J. J. (eds.), Analysis and Publication of Ceramics: The Computer Database in Archaeology, BAR International Series 551, British Archaeological Reports, Oxford, pp. 71–80.

  • Feathers, J. K. (1990). Explaining the Evolution of Ceramics in Southeast Missouri, Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Anthropology, University of Washington, Seattle.

  • Feldman, M. W., Aoki, K., and Kumm, J. (1996). Individual versus social learning: Evolutionary analysis in a fluctuating environment. Anthropological Science 104: 209–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, J. A. (1935). Ceramic Decoration Sequence at an Old Indian Village Site, Near Sicily Island, Louisiana, Anthropological Study No. 1, Department of Conservation, Louisiana Geological Survey, New Orleans.

  • Ford, J. A. (1936). Analysis of Indian Village Site Collections from Louisiana and Mississippi, Anthropological Study No. 2, Department of Conservation, Louisiana Geological Survey, New Orleans.

  • Ford, J. K. B. (1991). Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal waters of British Columbia. Canadian Journal of Zoology 69: 1454–1483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fritz, J., and Kotraschal, K. (1999). Social learning in common ravens (Corvus corax). Animal Behavior 57: 785–793.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fromkin, V., and Rodman, R. (1997). An Introduction to Language, 6th ed., Harcourt Brace, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gabora, L. M. (2000). The beer can theory of creativity. In Bentley, P., and Corne, D. (eds.), Creative Evolutionary Systems, Morgan Kaufman, San Francisco, pp. 1–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gabora, L. M. (2004). Ideas are not replicators but minds are. Biology and Philosophy 19: 127–143.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gell-Mann, M., and Lloyd, S. (2003). Effective Complexity, Santa Fe Institute Working Paper 03-12-068, Santa Fe, NM.

  • Gibbs, W. W. (2003). The unseen genome: Gems among the junk. Scientific American 289: 46–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grant, P., and Grant, R. (1996). Cultural inheritance of song and its role in the evolution of Darwin’s finches. Evolution 50: 2471–2487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hahn, M. W., and Bentley, R. A. (2003). Drift as a mechanism for cultural change: An example from baby names. Proceedings of the Royal Society Biology Letters B270: S120–S123.

  • Harpole, J., and Lyman, R. L. (2002). Changes in the fashion of women’s bonnets, 1831–1895. Missouri Archaeologist 63: 21–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatch, E. (1973). Theories of Man and Culture, Columbia University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayden, B. (ed.) (1979). Lithic Use-Wear Analysis, Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2001). Cultural transmission and the diffusion of innovations: Adoption dynamics indicate that biased cultural transmission is the predominant force in behavioral change. American Anthropologist 103: 992–1013.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2004a). Cultural group selection, coevolutionary processes and large-scale cooperation. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 53: 3–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2004b). Demography and cultural evolution: Why adaptive cultural processes produced maladaptive losses in Tasmania. American Antiquity 69: 197–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., and Boyd, R. (1998). The evolution of conformist transmission and between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior 19: 215–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., and Boyd, R. (2001). Why people punish defectors: Conformist transmission stabilizes costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. Journal of Theoretical Biology 208: 79–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., and Boyd, R. (2002). Culture and cognition: Why cultural evolution does not require replication of representations. Culture and Cognition 2: 87–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., and Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior 22: 165–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., and McElreath, R. (2003). The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Archaeology 12: 123–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herzog, H. A., Bentley, R. A., and Hahn, M. W. (2004). Random drift and large shifts in popularity of dog breeds. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences 271: S353–S356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. M., and Galef, J. B. G. (eds.) (1996). Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture, Academic Press, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, J. N. (1966). A prehistoric community in eastern Arizona. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology 22: 9–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, J. N. (1968). Broken K Pueblo: Patterns of form and function. In Binford, S. R., and Binford, L. R. (eds.), New Perspectives in Archeology, Aldine, Chicago, pp. 103–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, W. H. (1886). Ancient Pottery of the Mississippi Valley, Fourth Annual Report of the Bureau of Ethnology, 1882–83, Smithsonian Institution, Washington, DC, pp. 361–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Houston, S., Stuart, D., and Robertson, J. (1998). Disharmony in Maya hieroglyphic writing: Linguistic change and continuity in Classic society. In Ciudad Ruiz, A., Fernandez Marquinez, Y., Garcia Campillo, J. M., Iglesias Ponce de Leon, M. J., Garcia-Gallo, A. L., and Sanz Castro, L. T. (eds.), Anatomía de una civilización: aproximaciones interdisciplinarias a la cultura maya, Sociedad Española de Estudios Mayas, Madrid, pp. 275–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hruby, Z. X. (2002). Evidence for linguistic conservatism in the hieroglyphic script of the central Peten. Mayab 15: 49–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordan, P., and Shennan, S. (2003). Cultural transmission, language, and basketry traditions amongst the California Indians. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 22: 42–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kidder, A. V. (1915). Pottery of the Pajarito Plateau and some adjacent regions in New Mexico. In Memoirs of the American Anthropological Association, Vol. 2, American Anthropological Association, Lancaster, pp. 407–462.

  • Kidder, A. V. (1917). A design-sequence from New Mexico. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 3: 369–370.

  • Kohler, T. A., VanBuskirk, S., and Ruscavage-Barz, S. (2004). Vessels and villages: Evidence for conformist transmission in early village aggregations on the Pajarito Plateau, New Mexico. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 23: 100–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koppers, W. (1955). Diffusion: Transmission and acceptance. Yearbook of Anthropology 1955: 169–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kornbacher, K. (2001). Building components of evolutionary explanation: A study of wedge tools from northern South America. In Hunt, T., Lipo, C. P., and Sterling, S. (eds.), Posing Questions for a Scientific Archaeology, Bergin and Garvey, Westport, CT, pp. 23–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krieger, A. D. (1944). The typological concept. American Antiquity 3: 271–288.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroeber, A. L. (1916). Zuni Potsherds, Anthropological Paper No. 18, Part 1, American Museum of Natural History, New York.

  • Kroeber, A. L. (1919). On the principle of order in civilization as exemplified by changes of fashion. American Anthropologist 21: 235–263.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroeber, A. L. (1940). Statistical classification. American Antiquity 6: 29–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krützen, M., Mann, J., Heithaus, M. R., Connor, R. C., Bejder, L., and Sherwin, W. B. (2005). Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102: 8939–8943.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuijt, I. (2000). People and space in early agricultural villages: Exploring daily lives, community size and architecture in the late pre-pottery Neolithic. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 19: 75–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kujit, I. (2001). Place, death, and the transmission of social memory in early agricultural communities of the Near Eastern Pre-Pottery Neolithic. In Chesso, M. S. (ed.), Social Memory, Identity, and Death: Intradisciplinary Perspectives on Mortuary Rituals, American Anthropological Association, Washington, DC, pp. 80–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W. (1972). The social motivation of a sound change. In Labov, W. (ed.), Sociolinguistic Patterns, University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia, pp. 1–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Labov, W. (1994). Principles of Linguistic Change: Internal Factors, Basil Blackwell, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewontin, R. (1974). The Genetic Basis of Evolutionary Change, Columbia University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberson, S. (2000). A Matter of Taste: How Names, Fashions, and Culture Change, Yale University Press, New Haven, CT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipo, C. (2001a). Science, Style and the Study of Community Structure: An Example from the Central Mississippi River Valley, BAR International Series, No. 918, British Archaeological Reports, Oxford.

  • Lipo, C. (2001b). Community structures among late Mississippian populations of the central Mississippi River valley. In Hunt, T., Lipo, C. P., and Sterling, S. (eds.), Posing Questions for a Scientific Archaeology, Bergin and Garvey, Westport, CT, pp. 175–216

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipo, C., and Madsen, M. (2000). Neutrality, “style,” and drift: Building methods for studying cultural transmission in the archaeological record. In Hurt, T. D., and Rakita, G. F. M. (eds.), Style and Function: Conceptual Issues in Evolutionary Archaeology, Bergin and Garvey, Westport, CT, pp. 91–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipo, C., Madsen, M., Dunnell, R., and Hunt, T. (1997). Population structure, cultural transmission, and frequency seriation. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 16: 301–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Longacre, W. A. (1970). Archaeology as Anthropology: A Case Study, University of Arizona Press, Tucson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lumsden, C. J., and Wilson, E. O. (1981). Genes, Mind and Culture, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R. L. (2000). Culture historical and biological approaches to identifying homologous traits. In Hurt, T. D., and Rakita, G. F. M. (eds.), Style and Function. Conceptual Issues in Evolutionary Archaeology, Bergin and Garvey, Westport, CT, pp. 69–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R., and O’Brien, M. (2000). Measuring and explaining change in artifact variation with clade-diversity diagrams. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 19: 39–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R. L., and O’Brien, M. J. (2003). Cultural traits: Units of analysis in early twentieth-century anthropology. Journal of Anthropological Research 59: 225–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyman, R. L., O’Brien, M. J., and Dunnell, R. C. (1997). The Rise and Fall of Culture History, Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, A. (1996). The population memetics of birdsong. In Kroodsma, D. E., and Miller, E. H. (eds.), Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds, Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY, pp. 181–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacDonald, D. (1998). Subsistence, sex, and cultural transmission in Folsom culture. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 17: 217–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, E. (1959). Where are we. Cold Spring Harbor Symposium in Quantitative Biology 24: 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBrearty, S., Bishop, L., Plummer, T., Dewar, R., and Conard, N. (1998). Tools underfoot: Human trampling as an agent of lithic artifact edge modification. American Antiquity 63: 108–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • McClure, S. B. (2004). Cultural Transmission of Ceramic Technology During the Consolidation of Agriculture in Valencia, Spain, Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Anthropology, University of California, Santa Barbara, University Microfilms, Ann Arbor.

  • McElreath, R., Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (2003). Shared norms and the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology 44: 122–129.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGrew, W. (1992). Chimpanzee Material Culture. Implications for Human Evolution, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meggers, B., Evans, C., and Estrada, B. (1965). Early Formative Period of Coastal Ecuador: The Valdivia and Machalilla Phases, Smithsonian Contributions to Anthropology I, Smithsonian Institution, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzer, D. J. (1981). A study of style and function in a class of tools. Journal of Field Archaeology 8: 313–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., and Whiten, A. (2004). The hierarchical transformation of event knowledge in human cultural transmission. Journal of Cognition and Culture 4: 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., and Dunbar, R. I. M. (2006). A bias for social information in human cultural transmission. British Journal of Psychology 97: 405–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mithen, S. J. (1997). Cognitive archaeology, evolutionary psychology, and cultural transmission with particular reference to religious ideas. In Barton, C. M., and Clark, G. A. (eds.), Rediscovering Darwin: Evolutionary Theory in Archaeological Explanation, Archeological Paper No. 7, American Anthropological Association, Washington, DC, pp. 67–74.

  • Mithen, S. J. (1998). A creative explosion? Theory of mind, language, and the disembodied mind of the Upper Paleolithic. In Mithen, S. J. (ed.), Creativity in Human Evolution and Prehistory, Routledge, London, pp. 165–191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neiman, F. (1995). Stylistic variation in evolutionary perspective: Inferences from decorative diversity and interassemblage distance in Illinois Woodland ceramic assemblages. American Antiquity 60: 7–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, N. C. (1916). Chronology of the Tanos ruins, New Mexico. American Anthropologist 18: 159–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nishida, T. (1968). The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahali Mountains. Primates 9: 167–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norwich, K. H. (1983). On the theory of Weber fractions. Perception and Psychophysics 42: 286–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., and Holland, T. D. (1990). Variation, selection, and the archaeological record. In Schiffer, M. B. (ed.), Archaeological Method and Theory, Vol. 2, University of Arizona Press, Tucson, pp. 31–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., and Lyman, R. L. (1999). Seriation, Stratigraphy, and Index Fossils: The Backbone of Archaeological Dating, Kluwer Academic/Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., and Lyman, R. L. (2002). The epistemological nature of archaeological units. Anthropological Theory 2: 37–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., and Lyman, R. L. (2003). Style, function, transmission: An introduction. In O’Brien, M. J., and Lyman, R. L. (eds.), Style, Function, Transmission: Evolutionary Archaeological Perspectives, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City, pp. 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., Holland, T. D., Hoard, R. J., and Fox, G. L. (1994). Evolutionary implications of design and performance characteristics of prehistoric pottery. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 1: 259–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., Lyman, R. L., and Schiffer, M. B. (2005). Archaeology as a Process: Processualism and Its Progeny, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olsen, S. L., and Shipman, P. (1988). Surface modification on bone: Trampling versus butchery. Journal of Archaeological Science 15: 535–553.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orgel, L. E., and Crick, F. H. C. (1980). Selfish DNA: The ultimate parasite. Nature 284: 604–607.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. (1951). Culture: Its empirical and non-empirical character. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology 7: 202–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, C. T., and Wright, S. A. (1997). Categories and gatherings: Group selection and the mythology of cultural anthropology. Evolution and Human Behavior 18: 291–308.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, C. T., Coe, K., and Wadley, R. L. (2005). On tools and tradition. Current Anthropology 46: 459–460.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, C. T., Frederickson, B. E., and Tilley, C. F. (1995). On cultural group selection. Current Anthropology 36: 657–658.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrie, W. M. F. (1899). Sequences in prehistoric remains. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 29: 295–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfeffer, M. T. (2001). The engineering and evolution of Hawaiian fishhooks. In Hunt, T. L., Lipo, C. P., and Sterling, S. (eds.), Posing Questions for a Scientific Archaeology, Bergin and Garvey, Westport, CT, pp. 73–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillips, P., Ford, J. A., and Griffin, J. B. (1951). Archaeological Survey in the Lower Mississippi Alluvial Valley, 1940–1947, Papers of the Peabody Museum of American Archaeology and Ethnology 25, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.

  • Pocklington, R., and Best, M. L. (1997). Cultural evolution and units of selection in replicating text. Journal of Theoretical Biology 188: 79–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poole, J. H., Tyack, P. L., Stoeger-Horwath, A. S., and Watwood, S. (2005). Animal behaviour: Elephants are capable of vocal learning. Nature 434: 455–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Popper, K. (1959). The Logic of Scientific Discovery, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rendell, L., and Whitehead, H. (2001). Culture in whales and dolphins. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24: 309–382.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rickerson, P. J., and Boyd, R. (1978). A dual inheritance model of the human evolutionary process. Journal of Social and Biological Structures 1: 127–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, E. M. (2003). The Diffusion of Innovations, 5th ed., Free Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rouse, I. (1939). Prehistory in Haiti, Yale University Publications in Anthropology No. 21, New Haven, CT.

  • Rowlands, M. (1993). The role of memory in the transmission of culture. World Archaeology 25: 141–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B. (1987). Formation Processes of the Archaeological Record, University of New Mexico Press, Albuquerque.

  • Schiffer, M. B. (2004). Memes are a bad idea: Behavioral approaches to cultural transmission and evolution. Paper presented at the 68th Annual Meeting for the Society for American Archaeology, Montreal.

  • Schiffer, M. B., and Skibo, J. M. (1997). The explanation of artifact variability. American Antiquity 62: 27–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiffer, M. B., Skibo, J. M., Griffitts, J. L., Hollenback, K. L., and Longacre, W. A. (2001). Behavioral archaeology and the study of technology. American Antiquity 66: 729–737.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlag, K. H. (1998). Why imitate, and if so, how? Journal of Economic Theory 78: 130–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shannon, C. E., and Weaver, W. (1949). The Mathematical Theory of Communication, University of Illinois Press, Urbana.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheng-Wei, L., Jin-Jang, L., and Li-Wei, K. (2004). An error resilient coding scheme for H.26L video transmission based on data embedding. Journal of Visual Communication and Image Representation 15: 214–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. J. (1989). Cultural transmission and cultural change. In Van der Leeuw, S. E., and Torrance, R. (eds.), What’s New: A Closer Look at the Process of Innovation, Unwin Hyman, London, pp. 330–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. J. (2001). Demography and cultural innovation: A model and its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 11: 5–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. J. (2002). Genes, Memes, and Human History: Darwinian Archaeology and Cultural Evolution, Thames and Hudson, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. J., and Wilkinson, J. R. (2001). Ceramic style change and neutral evolution: A case study from Neolithic Europe. American Antiquity 66: 577–593.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shott, M. J. (1997). Stones and shafts redux: The metric discrimination of chipped-stone dart and arrow points. American Antiquity 62: 86–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skibo, J. M. (1992). Pottery Function: A Use-Alteration Perspective, Plenum Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slater, P. J. B., and Ince, S. A. (1979). Cultural evolution in chaffinch song. Behavior 71: 146–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, J. M., and Bell, P. A. (1994). Conformity as a determinant of behavior in a resource dilemma. Journal of Social Psychology 134: 191–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soltis, J., Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1995). Can group-functional behaviors evolve by cultural group selection? An empirical test. Current Anthropology 36: 473–494.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, C. (1993). Human agency, biased transmission, and the cultural evolution of chiefly authority. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 12: 41–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. (1996). Explaining Culture: A Naturalistic Approach, Blackwell, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spier, L. (1917). An Outline for a Chronology of Zuni Ruins, Anthropological Papers No. 18, Part 3, American Museum of Natural History, New York.

  • Tehrani, J., and Collard, M. (2002). Investigating cultural evolution through biological phylogenetic analyses of Turkmen textiles. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 21: 443–463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teltser, P. A. (1995). Culture history, evolutionary theory, and frequency seriation. In Teltser, P. A. (ed.), Evolutionary Archaeology: Methodological Issues, University of Arizona Press, Tucson, pp. 51–68

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., and Cosmides, L. (1992). Psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. (eds.), The Adapted Mind, Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 19–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trask, R. L. (1994). Language Change, Routledge, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Doorn, A. J., van de Grind, W. A., and Koenderink, J. J. (1984). Limits in Perception: Essays in Honour of Maarten A. Bouman, VNU Science Press, Utrecht, The Netherlands.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, R., Hong, F., and Chen, Z. (2004). Cryptography with no transmission error or cipher-text extension based on orthogonal table. Mini-Micro Systems 25: 1499–1501.

    Google Scholar 

  • Washburn, D. K. (2001). Remembering things seen: Experimental approaches to the process of information transmittal. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory 8: 67–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse, H. (1992). Memorable religions: Transmission, codification and change in divergent Melanesian contexts. Man 27: 777–799.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilhelmsen, K. H. (2001). Building the framework for an evolutionary explanation of projectile point variation: An example from the central Mississippi River valley. In Hunt, T. L., Lipo, C. P., and Sterling, S. (eds.), Posing Questions for a Scientific Archaeology, Bergin and Garvey, Westport, CT, pp. 97–144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woese, C. R. (2000). Interpreting the universal phylogenetic tree. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 97: 8392–8396.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woese, C. R. (2004). A new biology for a new century. Microbiology and Molecular Biology Reviews 68: 173–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wrangham, R., McGrew, W., de Waal, F., and Heltne, P. (1996). Chimpanzee Cultures, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

Bibliography of recent literature

  • Atran, S. (2001). The trouble with memes: Inference versus imitation in cultural creation. Human Nature 12: 351–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aunger, R. (2002). The Electric Meme: A New Theory of How We Think, Free Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barton, C. M., and Clark, G. A. (1997). Evolutionary theory and archaeological explanation. In Barton, C. M., and Clark, G. A. (eds.), Rediscovering Darwin: Evolutionary Theory and Archaeological Explanation, Archeological Papers Vol. 7, American Anthropological Association, Arlington, VA, pp. 3–15.

  • Beck, C. (1998). Projectile point types as valid chronological units. In Ramenofsky, A. F., and Steffen, A. (eds.), Unit Issues in Archaeology: Measuring Time, Space, and Material, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City, pp. 21–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentley, R. A. (2003). Cultural transmission and stochastic network growth. American Antiquity 68: 459–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Best, M. L., and Pocklington, R. (1999). Meaning as use: Transmission fidelity and evolution in netnews. Journal of Theoretical Biology 196: 389–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boone, J. L., and Smith, E. A. (1998). Is it evolution yet? A critique of evolutionary archaeology. Current Anthropology 39: 141–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1987). The evolution of ethnic markers. Cultural Anthropology 2: 65–79.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (1996). Why culture is common but cultural evolution is rare. Proceedings of the British Academy 88: 77–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., and Richerson, P. J. (2002). Group beneficial norms spread rapidly in a structured population. Journal of Theoretical Biology 215: 287–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer, P. (1999). Cognitive tracks of cultural inheritance: How evolved intuitive ontology governs cultural transmission. American Anthropologist 100: 876–889.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broughton, J. M., and O’Connell, J. F. (1999). On evolutionary ecology, selectionist archaeology, and behavioral archaeology. American Antiquity 64: 153–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwent, J., and O’Brien, M. J., (2005). Using cladistics to construct lineages of projectile points from northeastern Missouri. In Lipo, C. L., O’Brien, M. J., Collard, M., and Shennan, S. J. (eds.), Mapping Our Ancestors: Phylogenetic Approaches in Anthropology and Prehistory, Aldine, New York, pp. 185–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2001). Cultural transmission and the diffusion of innovations: Adoption dynamics indicate that biased cultural transmission is the predominate force in behavioral change. American Anthropologist 103: 992–1013.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., and Boyd, R. (2002). Culture and cognition: Why cultural evolution does not require replication of representations. Culture and Cognition 2: 87–112.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, G. T, Leonard, R. D., and Abbot, A. L. (1995). The structure of selectionist explanations in archaeology. In Teltser, P. A. (ed.), Evolutionary Archaeology: Methodological Issues, University of Arizona Press, Tucson, pp. 13–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lanata, J. L., and Neff, H. (1999). Discutiendo algunas escalas of transmision cultural: artefacto y espacio. Revista do Museo de Arquelogia y Etnologia (Sao Paulo), Supplemento 3: 46–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipo, C. L., O’Brien, M. J., Collard, M., and Shennan, S. J. (eds.) (2005). Mapping Our Ancestors: Phylogenetic Approaches in Anthropology and Prehistory, Aldine, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, A. (1996). Thought Contagion: How Belief Spreads Through Society, Basic Books, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., and Laland, K. N. (2004). Perspective: Is human cultural evolution Darwinian? Evidence reviewed from the perspective of the Origin of Species. Evolutionary Anthropology 58: 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neff, H. (2000). On evolutionary ecology and evolutionary archaeology: Some common ground? Current Anthropology 41: 427–429.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., and Lyman, R. L. (2000). Measuring and explaining change in artifact variation with clade-diversity diagrams. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 19: 39–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., and Lyman, R. L. (2004). History and explanation in archaeology. Anthropological Theory 4: 173–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, M. J., Darwent, J., and Lyman, R. L. (2001). Cladistics is useful for reconstructing archaeological phylogenies: Paleoindian points from the southeastern United States. Journal of Archaeological Science 28: 1115–1136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, C. (1998). Theory, measurement, and explanation, variable shapes in poverty point objects. In Ramenofsky, A. F., and Steffen, A. (eds.), Unit Issues in Archaeology: Measuring Time, Space, and Material, University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City, pp. 163–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., Boyd, R., and Henrich, J. (2003). The cultural evolution of human cooperation. In Hammerstein, P. (ed.), The Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp. 357–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. J. (2002). Learning. In Hart, J. P., and Terrell, J. E. (eds.), Darwin and Archaeology: A Handbook of Key Concepts, Bergin and Garvey, Westport, CT, pp. 183–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, G. (1996). Cultural Selection, BasicBooks, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tschauner, H. (1994). Archaeological systematics and cultural evolution: Retrieving the honour of culture history. Man 29: 77–93.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jelmer W. Eerkens.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Eerkens, J.W., Lipo, C.P. Cultural Transmission Theory and the Archaeological Record: Providing Context to Understanding Variation and Temporal Changes in Material Culture. J Archaeol Res 15, 239–274 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10814-007-9013-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10814-007-9013-z

Keywords

Navigation